Berber languages normally have three basic aspect/mood forms:
- the “aorist” (or “simple imperfect”), used mainly for hypothetical events (“eat!”, “I will eat”, “I would eat”...);
- the “preterite” (or “simple perfect”), used mainly for past events conceived of as wholes (“I ate”, “I have eaten”);
- the “intensive” (or “intensive imperfect”), used for events ongoing at the time being referred to, irrespective of tense (“I eat”, “I am eating”, “I was eating”, “keep eating!”)
- Aorist: ǎlməd “learn!”
- Preterite: (y)-əlmǎd “(he) learned” (change the vowel pattern)
- Intensive: (i-)lammǎd “he is learning” (double the middle consonant)
- Aorist: əlməd “learn!”
- Preterite: (y)-əlməd “(he) learned”
- Intensive: (i)-ləmməd “he is learning”
But some verbs have two consonants rather than three. Looking at Siwi I noticed that, if the verb had two consonants and no long vowels, there seemed to be two possibilities for the intensive, not just one; contrast:
- Aorist: fəl “leave!”
- Preterite: (y)-əfla “(he) left”
- Intensive: (i)-təffal “he is leaving”
- Aorist: ləs “wear!”
- Preterite: (y)-əlsa “(he) wore”
- Intensive: (i)-ləss “he is wearing”
Well, looking at the intensive forms, you see that in fəl you double the first consonant, while for ləs you double the second one. If you wanted to try to relate these to three-consonant verbs, you might think of something like:
- fəl < *Xfl
- ləs < *lsX
But if you look at Siwi on its own, there seem to be a lot of problems with this idea: in particular, why would the preterite of fəl end in -a?
Looking wider provides some answers. It turns out that in Tuareg – like Kabyle, and Tashelhiyt, and Ghadamsi, and a few other varieties – these verbs are distinct in the preterite too, and they are distinguished in exactly the way you'd expect from that little piece of internal reconstruction:
- Aorist: əfəl “leave!”; əǵən “kneel!”
- Preterite: (y)-fǎl “(he) left”; (y)-ǵǎn “(it) knelt”
- Intensive: (y)-ffal “he is leaving”; (y)-ǵǵan “it is kneeling”
- Aorist: ǎls “wear!”; əsəl "hear!"
- Preterite: (y)-lsa “(he) wore”; (y)-sla "he heard"
- Intensive: (y)-lass “he is wearing”; (y)-sall "he is hearing"
Zenaga, the Berber language of Mauritania, gives us part of the answer. In Zenaga, they look like this:
- Aorist: ägun “kneel!”
- Preterite: (y)-ugän “(it) knelt”
- Intensive: (y)-uggan / (yə)-ttugun “it is kneeling”
- Aorist: ätyši “wear!”, ätyšaʔ-m “wear! (to a group)”
- Preterite: (y)-ityša “(he) wore; ityšäʔ-n “they wore”
- Intensive: (yi)-yässä “he is wearing”; yässäʔ-n “they are wearing”
- Aorist: *ǎlsəʔ “wear!”
- Preterite: *(y)-əlsǎʔ “(he) wore”
- Intensive: *(yə)-lassǎʔ “he is wearing”
But there's still a problem here: why is *-ǎʔ reflected differently in the intensive vs. the preterite? A full answer for that would require a look at reflexes of the glottal stop in general, not just in the verbal system. But in several Berber languages, in fact, it's reflected identically. Compare, from opposite ends of the Berber world:
Tashelhiyt (southern Morocco):
- Aorist: ls “wear!”
- Preterite: (i)-lsa “(he) wore”
- Intensive: (i)-lssa “he is wearing”
- Aorist: əsəl “hear!”
- Preterite: (yə)-sla “(he) heard”
- Intensive: (i)-səlla “he is hearing”
* (All but the last bit of this post is an introductory summary of work by Prasse, Kossmann, and Taine-Cheikh that I've recently been digesting. It offers an interesting small-scale parallel to the story of Saussure's laryngeals.)
3 comments:
Great to see you delve into this subject!
Just something I'd like to add was the following.
I think fel is not of the type Xfl, or at least not of the type ʔFL. Verbs that have an initial gottal stop, generally retain a stable long vowel in the other Berber Languages
For example Taine-Cheikh's root ʔGH/H* `to refuse'(page 20)
A yoʔgih P yuʔgäh
This corresponds perfectly with Tamazight GY `to refuse'
A yagey P yugey
Proto-Berber would be:
A *äʔgëy P *ëʔgäy
(*ë for schwa, *ä for short a)
The place of the glottal stop may have been before the vowel, but due to Zénaga evidence this is hard to determine. But I have trouble understanding how *ʔä/*ʔë would yield a/u. But maybe that's my Indo-Europeanist mind wanting to find *VH > VV
I think the loss of y in Zénaga is regular in this context, but the reflexes word finally can be very confusing.
So then what was fel?
I think that we should not look for a third consonant in this root.
A *fël(?) P *fäl I *ffal
I don't understand the *a infix in the Intensive (I'd expect ffël). But the long f can be explained by changing the rule to 'The before last consonant geminates' rather than 'the middle consonant geminates'.
(I'm sorry if this is a double post, Blogger was giving me errors)
I agree that certainly the X in "fel" is not from a glottal stop - that would have shown up in Zenaga.
Prasse suggests the structure fXl for "fel" etc instead. At first I thought this was mad, but the more I think about the better it sounds. The cluster would explain why the initial vowel shows up in the imperative etc., while intensive fǎXXǎl > fal makes good sense. Then instead of the vowel length being irregular, it's the gemination of f that's irregular, and we have to assume that it was motivated by a secondary application of a rule like the one you suggest - "geminate the penultimate consonant in the intensive".
Now in general fel-type verbs do not have a glottal stop in Zenaga. But there is one odd, apparently irregular exception:
- Aorist: aḍuṛ “fall!”
- Preterite: (y)-uḍar “(he) fell”
- Intensive: (y)-uḍḍaʔṛ “he is falling”
We could explain this by supposing that there is a contrast between *fhl "go" and *ḍʔr "fall", if we assume that even in Zenaga ʔ > 0 / C_, or at least C_[short vowel].
interesting! But how do you know "lemed" (study/learn) is a Phoenician word?
Post a Comment